Minggu, 27 April 2014

Natural classes


Suppose you were writing a grammar of English and wished to include all the generalities that children acquire about the set of phonemes and their allophones. One way of showing what speakers of the language know about the predictable aspects of speech is to include these generalities as phonological rules in the phonological component of the grammar. These are not the rules that someone teaches you at school or that you must obey because someone insists on it; they are rules that are known unconsciously and that express the phonological regularities of the language.
 
In English phonology, such rules determine the conditions under which vowels are nasalised or voiceless stops are aspirated. They are general rules, applying not to a single sound but to classes of sounds. They also apply to all the words in the vocabulary of the language, and they even apply to nonsense words that are not in the language but could enter the language (such as sint, peeg, or sprag, which would be /sɪnt/, /pig/, and /spag/ phonemically and [si`nt], [phig], and [spag] phonetically).

There are also less general rules found in all languages and there may also be exceptions to these general rules. But what is of greater interest is that the more we examine the phonologies of the many thousands of languages of the world, the more we find similar phonological rules that apply to the same board general classes of sounds, like the one we have mentioned nasals, voiceless stpos, alveolars, labials, and so on.

For example, many languages of world include the rule that nasalises vowels before nasal consonants. One need not include a list of the individual sounds to which the rule applies or the sounds which result from its application. We can state the rule as :

Nasalise a vowel when it is followed by a nasal consonant in the sanme syllable.
This rule will apply to all vowel phonemes when they occur in a context before any segment marked [+ nasal], and will add the feature [+ nasal] to the feature matrix of the vowels.

Another rule that occurs frequently in the world’s languages changes the place of articulation of nasal consonants to the place of articulation of the following consonant. Thus, an /n/ will become an [m] before before a /p/ or /b/ and will become a velar [ŋ] before a /k/ or /g/. When two segments agree in their place of articulation they are called homorganic consonants. This homorganic nasal rules occur in Akan as well as English and many other languages.

Many languages have rules which refer to [+ voiced] and [- voiced] sounds. Note that the aspiration rule in English applies to the class of voiceless stops. As in the vowel nasality rule, we did not list the individual segments in the rule since it applies to all the voiceless stops /p, t, k/, as well as to /tʃ/.

That we find such similar rules which apply to the same classes of sounds across languages is not surprising since such rules often have phonetic explanations and these classes of sounds are defined by phonetic features. For this reason, such classes are called natural classes of speech sounds. A natural class is a group of sounds that share one or more distinctive features.

Children find it easier to learn a rule (or construct one) that applies to a natural class of sounds; they do not have to remember the individual sounds, simply the features that these sounds share.

This fact about phonological rules and natural classes illustrates why individual phonemis segments are better regarded as combinations or complexes of features than as indissolube whole segments. If such segments are not be revealed. It would appear that it should be just as easy for a child to learn a rule such as

1. Nasalise vowels before /p/, /i/, or /z/
As to learn a rule such as
2. Nasalise vowels before /m/, /n/, or /ŋ/

Rule (1) has no phonetic explanation whereas rule (2) does. It is easier to raise the velum to produce a nasalised vowel in anticipation of a following nasal consonant than to prevent the velum from rising before the consonant closure.

A natural classes is a set of phonemes that can be defineed by fewer features than any of its members. The classes that include the phoneme /p, t, k, b, g, m, n, ŋ, tʃ, dʒ/ can be defined specifying one feature, [ - continuant]. The phoneme /p/ requires three feature specifications, as does any of the other phonemes in this set.

A class of sounds that can be defined by fewer features than another class of sounds is clearly more general. Thus, the class of [ - continuant, - nasal] sounds in some sense more natural than the class that includes all the segments in that class. Try to do this with feature notation: you will see why such a class is far from natural.

This does not mean that no language has a rule which applies to single sound or even to a class of stops excluding /p/. One does find complex rules in languages including rules that apply to an individual member of a class, but rules pretaining to natural classes occur more frequently, and an explanation is provided for this fact by reference to phonetic properties.

A phonological segment may be a member of a number of classes; for example, /s/ is a member of the class of [+ obstruent]s, [consonants]s, [+ alveolar]s, [+ coronal]s, [- stop]s, [+ continuant]s, [+ sibiliant]s, and so on.