Minggu, 27 April 2014

More on prosodic phonology



Intonation
In chapter 6, the use of pitch as a phonetic feature was discussed in reference to tone languages and intonation languages. In this chapter we have disscused the use of phonetic features to distinguish meaning. We can now see that pitch can be a phonetic feature in languages such as Chinese, or Thai, or Akan. Such relative pitch are referred to phonologically as contrasting tones. We also pointed out that there are languages that are not tone languages, such as English. Pitch may still play an important role. It is the pitch contour or intonation of the phrase or sentence that is important.

In English, syntactic differences may be shown by different intonation contours. We say John is going as a statement with a falling pitch, but as a question with the pitch rising at the end of the sentence.
A sentence which is ambiguous when it is written may be unambiguous when spoken. For example :

1. Tristram left direction for Isolde to follow.
If Tristram wanted Isolde to follow him, the sentence would be pronounced with the rise in pitch on the first syllable of follow, fllowed by a fall in pitch, as in (2)

2. Tristram left directions for Isolade to follow.
The sentence can also mean that Tristram left a set of directions he wanted Isolde to use. If this is the intention, the highest pitch comes on the second syllable of directions as in (3) :

3. Tristram left directions for Isolde to follow.
The way we have indicated pitch is of course highly oversimplified. Before the big rise in pitch the voice does not remain on the same low pitch. These diagrams indicate merely when there is a special change in pitch.

The primary function of intonation is to indicate sentence meaning, but many linguist would also say that it has other functions. One is to signal the attitude of the speaker. Feelings of anger, impatience, relief, surprise, and so on, can all be expressed through intonation as well as by the words we use. For example, the well-known Australian habit of ending a statement with a rise in pitch indicates uncertainly usually a desire on the part of the speaker to check that the message has been understood.
Although we talk about intonation solely in terms of pitch contour, or direction, there are other pitch features that affect our preception of sentences. Pitch range (the degree of difference between the high pitches and the low pitches) indicate emotional involvement. The range tends to widen with excitement, and to narrow with boredom or wearniess. Pitch height tends to vary with the familiarity of he context; the average pitch of the voice is often lower in more intimate situations.
Such variation in intonation is often parallelled by a difference in other prosodic feature such as loudness; just as voice that is soft and low can signal warm friendliness, loud tones with a raised pitch will often interpreted as overbearing or aggresive. Rate of utterence serves several function. Variation in the speed of talking may mark grammatical boundaries, indicate parenthetical statements (‘Raelene’s boyfriend you know the one i mean called over last nigh’), or may, like the other features, show differences in the intensity of feeling.
Sentence (4) can display a range of attitudes or ‘meaning’ depending on the prosodic features which are associated with the words :
(4) Come here
Varying degrees of loudness, pitch range and height, combined with either fast or slow utterance, will make the sentence sound peremptory, quietly menacing, or resigned in tone, or could turn it into an intimate invitation. All these can be expressed even though the pitch contour retains its standard falling pattern.

Word stress
In English and many other languages, one or more of the syllables in each content word (words other the ‘little words’ such as to, the, a, of, and so on) are stressed. The stressed sylable is marked by an acute accent ( ’) in the following examples :
 

These minimal pairs show that is contrastive in English; it distinguishes between nouns and verbs.
In some words, more than one vowel is tressed, but if so, one of these stressed vowels receives greater stress than the others. We have indicated the most highly stressed vowel by an acute accent ( ’) over the vowel (we say this vowel receives the accent, or primary stress, or main stress); the other stressed vowels are indicated by marking a grave (`) over the vowels (these vowels receive secondary stress).
 
Generally, speakers of a language know which syllable receives primary stress or accent, which receives secondary stress, and which syllables are not stressed at all; it is part of their knowledge of the language. Sometimes it is hard to distinguish between primary and secondary stress, but it is easier to distinguish between stressed and unstressed syllables.
 
The stress pattern of a word may differ from dialect to dialect. For example, in British and Australasian English the word labo`ratory has only one stressed syllable; in most varieties of American English it has two. Because the vowel qualities in English are closely related to whether they are stressed or not, the British and Australasian vowels differ from the American vowels in this word; in fact, the American version has an ‘extra’ vowel because of the way it is stressed.
Just as stressed syllables in poetry reveal the metrical structure of the verse, phonological stress patterns relate to the metrical structure of a language.

We can than, specify which syllable in the word is stressed by marking the vowel in that syllable (because vowels constitute the syllable peaks) as either [+ stress] or [- stress]. We can also designate stress by numbers. The primary stressed or accented vowel can be designated by placing a ‘1’ over the vowels; secondary stress can be designated by a ‘2’; unstressed vowels are left unmarked.

Alternatively, we can use a system popular in many dictionaries (including the Macquarie Dictionary): high and low vertical bars indicate the two levels of stress.

Stress is property of a syllable rather than a segment, so it is a prosodic or suprasegnental feature. Tone may also be a property of a syllable rather than a single vowel; it, too, then, would be a suprasegmental feature.

To produce a stressed syllable, we may change the pitch (usually by raising it), make the syllable louder, or make it longer. We often use all three of these phonetic features to stress a syllable.

Sentence and phrase stress
When words are combined into phrases and sentences, one of the syllables receives greater stress than all other. That is, just as there is only one primary stress in a word spoken in isolation (foe example, in a list), only one of the vowels in a phrase (or sentence) receives primary stress or accent; all the other stressed vowels are ‘reduced’ to secondary stress. A syllable that receives the main stress when the word is not in a phrase may have only secondary stress in a phrase. Is illustrated by these examples :

In English, we place primary stress on an adjective followed by a noun when the two words are combined in a compound noun (usually, but not always, written as one sound), but we place the stress on the noun when the words are not joined in this way. The differences between the pairs above are therefore predictable :

These minimal pairs show that stress may be predictable if phonological rules include non-phonological information; that is, the phonology is not independent of the rest of the grammar. The stress differences between the noun and verb pairs (foe example, subject as noun and verb) discussed in the previous section are also predictable from the word category.
In the English sentences we used to illustrate intonation contours, we may also describe the differences by referring to the word on which the main stress is placed, as in the following examples :

1 Tristram left directions for Isolde to follow.
2. Tristram left directions for Isolde to follow.

In sentence (1) the primary stress is on the word follow, and in sentence (2) the primary stress is on directions.